Powerful Products for Powerful Results
ALCHEMY
TRAINING
We piloted Ron Cole’s SuperReading © course at LLU+, London Southbank University, with 15 adult dyslexic readers from January 2008. Despite my initial scepticism, both speed of reading and comprehension increased for all participants. In effect, reading effectiveness (including my own) doubled over the six sessions of the course (spread over 10 weeks). This was a pilot study to establish whether the claimed effect of the intervention appeared to work. The effect was dramatic and has been duplicated since with further readers. The pilot research is to be published in the peer reviewed Journal of Inclusive Learning in FE & HE (Sept 2009a). Since the pilot research, we have sought funds to finance further research involving control groups. Without this, it is not possible to establish with any degree of certainty, what is having the effect. Nevertheless, despite the initial relatively small group size, the effect was so large that the statistical significance of the effect was extremely high (p<0.001) and increasing with each further course as the sample size increases (currently less than 1 chance in a million that it could occur simply by chance).
Perhaps the most interesting feature of the effect is that those with the most difficulty reading nonwords make the most dramatic gains. This is despite the intervention involving no phonics. It involves metacognitive reading skills and an eye exercise designed to improve the visual processing of groups of words at speed. This effect cannot be explained away by ‘regression to the mean’, meaning that those with the weakest reading skills are making the most apparent gains as they move towards average reading skills, for two reasons. The first is that those with the most difficulty reading nonwords did not have the poorest reading skills in the dyslexic group. (We usually find that adult dyslexic readers who struggle with phonic attack develop very good reading for meaning strategies to compensate for difficulties with unfamiliar words allied with visual recognition of the meaning of words). Secondly, their reading skills generally developed beyond the mean, as we shall see below.
Perhaps the most obvious possible explanation for the effect is an inaccuracy in the measurement of it. However, unlike some recent research into phonics interventions with adults (Burton et al, 2009) which used the same tests before and after the intervention, we used different texts. The set of texts and questions the participant took before, during and after the intervention were randomised, so that differences in the difficulty or familiarity of a specific text or questions could not be an explanation for any differences across the group. In addition, standardised scores on TOWRE sight word tests increased by a mean of 7 standardised points (Cooper, 2009a), indicating that visual recognition of single words at speed improved (even though the intention was to improve the processing of multiple words, rather than single words).
We began to be left with very few possible explanations for the effect other than the intervention. We decided to see if the effect was duplicated when Ron Cole was not teaching the course, in case it was a consequence of his charisma. He trained a number of us to teach it, which we have begun to do. We also felt that this would increase our capacity if we could find the funds to finance larger scale research. This is where I feel that the results became even more interesting.
After the intervention, the mean reading speeds and comprehension of the dyslexic readers exceed the mean reading speeds and comprehension of the non-
Measuring Reading Effectiveness
Two measures of both reading speed and comprehension are taken at each point in time. Participants are given a text of four hundred words to read in order to answer unseen questions. The reading speed is measured. The text is then removed and 10 questions provided. These questions are not multiple choice (which can generate false positives) and are designed to reduce the effect of prior knowledge by making sure that they focus on details in the text that are unlikely to be known prior to reading it. This means that the focus is on detail (and recall of the detail read) rather than general knowledge and inference. (This attention to detail and recall is usually particularly difficult for dyslexic readers and is likely, therefore, to underestimate the impact of the intervention on real reading where details can simply be looked up). Once completed, the questions and the first set of attempted answers are removed and the same text provided with the invitation to read it again to attempt to get 100% of the answers correct. This review reading speed is measured. Once answers to the questions are again attempted, comprehension for each set of answers is scored as a percentage.
Reading Effectiveness (RE) is measured for both the ‘first read’ and the ‘review read’ by multiplying the words per minute with the percentage of correct answers. So, for example, if the reader read at 200 wpm, and got 50% of the correct answers, the RE score is 200×50/100=100. If they read at 100wpm and got 80% of the correct answers, the RE score is 100×80/100=80. In this way, RE scores the effectiveness of reading for comprehension within a timescale. RE scores would increase if just speed or comprehension increased. However, both speed and comprehension increased for all the participants.
It was suggested by one of the participants that perhaps practicing the comprehension tests itself leads to the improvement. In the absence of a control group, we found 11 volunteers to take the comprehension tests as many times as the course participants in as close to the same circumstances as we could provide. Overall, neither their reading speeds, nor comprehension improved at all.
Making comparisons
We now have a larger body of participants and can begin to make comparisons based on estimated standard deviations and mean scores. While this is no substitute for large scale independent research including control groups, it can provide very interesting perspectives on the comparative improvement of dyslexic, compared to non-
One standard deviation below the mean is currently used as the cut-
For each measurement I shall provide the mean score of the dyslexic participants (n=20) before the intervention, compared with the mean score of the non-
Overall comparisons
Before looking at detail at each measure of speed and comprehension, I shall present an overview of the impact on participants through looking at ‘combined RE’ scores. This is a convenient way of comparing the total time taken from both reading and reviewing for the final comprehension. It is measured by adding the time taken for both sets of reading, divided by the number of words (giving a ‘total wpm’ score) multiplied by the percentage comprehension achieved after the review. This allows us to measure the combined effect of both sets of reading. It measures total comprehension scores against total time taken, thus providing the best overall comparative measure.
Mean combined RE
MEAN COMBINED RE % ABOVE THE MEAN % BELOW 1.ST DEV BELOW MEAN % ABOVE 1 ST. DEV ABOVE MEAN
Pre-
Pre-
Pre-
The estimated standard deviation is 38. The improvement made by the dyslexic group is 55. Here we can see that the dyslexic group have progressed from being significantly weaker in their reading effectiveness than the non-
The strategies taught on the SuperReading course of previewing, questioning text, using eye-
First Reading Speed
MEAN WMP % ABOVE THE MEAN % BELOW 1.ST DEV BELOW MEAN % ABOVE 1 ST. DEV ABOVE MEAN
Pre-
Pre-
Pre-
The estimated standard deviation is 75 wpm. The mean increase for the dyslexic group is 43 wpm. Both groups show that most of the participants score below the mean, which means that a few readers are much faster than the others, although the percentage above one standard deviation above the mean remains small. Following the intervention, the dyslexic group mean score exceeds the mean reading speed of the non-
MEAN COMPREHENSION % ABOVE THE MEAN % BELOW 1.ST DEV BELOW MEAN % ABOVE 1 ST. DEV ABOVE MEAN
Pre-
Pre-
Pre-
Mean improvement of the dyslexic group is 12%. Again, the dyslexic group have improved from being significantly less able to answer comprehension questions at the first read to being quite similar to the non-
First Reading RE
MEAN RE % ABOVE THE MEAN % BELOW 1.ST DEV BELOW MEAN % ABOVE 1 ST. DEV ABOVE MEAN
Pre-
Pre-
Pre-
The estimated standard deviation is 45. The mean improvement of the dyslexic group is 44. Since reading quickly can reduce comprehension, while reading more slowly can, all things remaining equal, improve comprehension scores, these RE scores are more significant than just the speed or comprehension scores alone, since they measure reading comprehension against time. As we can see, the post intervention dyslexic mean score exceeds the non-
Review Reading Speed
MEAN WMP % ABOVE THE MEAN % BELOW 1.ST DEV BELOW MEAN % ABOVE 1 ST. DEV ABOVE MEAN
Pre-
Pre-
Pre-
The estimated standard deviation is 119 wpm. The mean improvement of the dyslexic group is 225 wpm (more than doubling their mean reading speed while also, as we can see later, improving their comprehension). Here we can see a large improvement over the non-
Review Reading comprehension
MEAN COMPREHENSION % ABOVE THE MEAN % BELOW 1.ST DEV BELOW MEAN % ABOVE 1 ST. DEV ABOVE MEAN
Pre-
Pre-
Pre-
The estimated standard deviation is 13%. The mean improvement of the dyslexic group is 18%. Again, the dyslexic group have improved significantly beyond the non-
Review Reading RE
MEAN RE % ABOVE THE MEAN % BELOW 1.ST DEV BELOW MEAN % ABOVE 1 ST. DEV ABOVE MEAN
Pre-
Pre-
Pre-
The estimated standard deviation is 124. The mean improvement of the dyslexic group is 233 (almost trebling their score). Here, the improved dyslexic scores are far superior to the non-
Conclusions
Although we are still seeking funds for larger scale research involving control groups, the evidence of an effect is compelling. Perhaps most tellingly, overall across all scores measured, 35% of the dyslexic group converted one or more score from one standard deviation below the mean prior to the intervention, to one standard deviation above the mean after the intervention; more than two estimated standard deviations.
To put all this data in context, if an average book is 120,000 words, prior to the intervention it was taking the dyslexic group almost 25 hours to gain a 73% c
omprehension/recall level. This compared with the non-
However, it is the implications of the effect that are most far reaching. The Rose Review (2009) of dyslexia has based most of its demanded phonics intervention on the assumption that phonics intervention is a necessary prerequisite for improving literacy. Singleton (2009) argues that there is no research in peer reviewed journals to support ‘reading recovery’ methods with dyslexic learners and (perhaps forgetting that a lack of counter-
References
Burton, M et al (2009) Burton, Davey, Lewis, Ritchie & Brooks (2008) Improving Reading: Phonics and Fluency. NRDC.
Cooper, R (2td class=”title” style=”border: 2px; border-
Cooper, R (2009b) Dyslexia, in Pollak, D (Ed.) Neurodiversity in Higher Education; Positive responses to specific learning differences, Wiley-
Report to Patoss on SuperReading and Dyslexia
We’re a small UK-
ALCHEMY
TRAINING
GET IN TOUCH
-
UK Phone: +44 (0)7738 666511
Email: info@alchemy.name
Skype: roncole42
THE PROFESSIONAL ASSOCIATION OF TEACHERS OF STUDENTS WITH SPECIFIC LEARNING DISABILITIES
Patoss
This report was submitted to Patoss for publication.
Patoss DOES NOT endorse or recognise The SuperReading Program.
This report was submitted to Patoss for publication.
Patoss DOES NOT endorse or recognise The SuperReading Program.